Skip to main content

Analysis of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory behaviour in mice lacking Nfix from adult neural stem cells

Abstract

Objective

The active place avoidance task (APA) is a behavioural task used to assess learning and memory in rodents. This task relies on the hippocampus, a region of the cerebral cortex capable of generating new neurons from neural stem cells. In this study, to gain further insight into the behavioural phenotype of mice deficient in the transcription factor Nfix, a gene expressed by adult neural stem cells, we examined learning and memory parameters from the APA task that were not published in our original investigation. We analysed time to first and second shock, maximum path and time of shock avoidance, number of entries into the shock zone and time spent in the shock zone. We also assessed performance in the APA task based on sex.

Results

We found mice deficient in Nfix displayed decreased latency to second shock compared to the control mice. Nfix deficient mice entered the shock zone more frequently and also spent more time in the shock zone. Our data provides further insights into the memory deficits evident in Nfix mutant mice, indicating these mice have a memory retrieval problem and may employ a different navigation strategy in the APA task.

Introduction

The active place avoidance task (APA) is a well-established method for assessing learning and memory in rodents [1,2,3,4,5]. This task is designed to test spatial learning and memory in mice by evaluating the ability of the animal to avoid a fixed shock zone relative to external spatial cues, a task that requires the hippocampus. Mice are placed on a slowly rotating platform surrounded by external cues that are used to delineate the position of a fixed shock zone. As the platform is continually moving, mice must integrate conflicting positional information cues with the external positional cues to avoid the shock zone [2, 5].

Successful avoidance of the shock zone in the APA task is dependent on the generation of adult born neurons [6]. We have previously shown that in mice where the transcription factor nuclear factor one X (Nfix) is specifically ablated from adult hippocampal stem cells (referred to as NfixiNestin mice [7]), intermediate neuronal progenitors fail to differentiate, resulting in fewer adult-born hippocampal neurons generated, leading to deficits in learning and memory [7]. We showed that NfixiNestin mice receive significantly more shocks than control littermates in the APA task. Following our initial study, we further analysed our data, evaluating the latency to first and second shock. A decreased latency to first or second shock would be suggestive of a long-term memory retrieval problem [8], thus providing further insight into the learning and memory capabilities of NfixiNestin mice. In addition, we evaluated other learning and memory parameters from the APA task, including maximum time of shock avoidance, maximum path of shock avoidance, number of entries into the shock zone and time spent in the shock zone. We also analysed the performance of the mice based on sex, as our initial study utilised both male and female mice, which differs from previous studies using APA [2, 6, 8, 9]. Here, we show that there was no difference in the total number of shocks received by male or female mice in either control or NfixiNestin mice. We reveal NfixiNestin mice are capable of spatial learning, as demonstrated by their similar maximum time and distance travelled to avoid a shock compared to control mice. Rather, the disparate performance of NfixiNestin mice compared to controls was evidenced by more frequent entries into the shock zone, increased time spent in the shock zone and decreased latency to second shock. Thus, our analysis reveals NfixiNestin mice have altered learning capabilities, perhaps adopting different navigation strategies to perform the APA task.

Main text

Methods

Animals and tamoxifen treatment

Conditional Nfix mice (Nfixf/f) harbour loxP sites flanking exon 2 [10, 11]. These mice were crossed to the nestin-creERT2 strain [12] to generate homozygous NfixiNestin mice. All mice were maintained on a C57BL/6J genetic background, and had been backcrossed for 10 generations. Loss-of-function animals were homozygous NfixiNestin mice that were given tamoxifen dissolved in corn-oil. Control mice were cre-negative NfixControl mice treated with tamoxifen. Tamoxifen administration was performed as previously described [7]. Adult (8–10 week old) male and female mice were used in this study, assessed 45 days after tamoxifen administration. A total of 21 mice of each genotype was used. After behavioural testing, mice were anaesthetised, then were perfused transcardially with phosphate buffered saline (10 ml), then with 4% paraformaldehyde (30 ml).

Active place avoidance

The active place avoidance (APA) task was used to assess hippocampal-dependent spatial learning [4, 5]. Prior to testing, mice were handled by the researcher for 2–3 min for three consecutive days. The following day, mice were habituated to the behavioural arena for 5 min, with no shocks administered during this time (shock zone was inactive). The behavioural arena consisted of a grid floor (grid spacing 0.5 cm, total diameter 77 cm, Bio-Signal group). This grid is placed upon a base that has a motor and swivel to allow for the entire apparatus to rotate clockwise at 1 revolution/min. A 32 cm high clear plastic circular fence defined the perimeter of the arena. Four contrasting visual cues were placed equally on the walls of the testing room (approximate distance to visual cues from the arena perimeter is 45 cm). To commence a trial, the mouse was placed in the arena facing the wall directly opposite the designated shock zone, which did not change throughout the experiment. Once the mouse was placed in the arena the trial was started and rotation of the arena began. The position of the mouse in the arena was tracked by an overhead camera linked to Tracker software (Bio-Signal group, version 2.36). When the mouse entered the 60 degree shock zone, a 500 ms, 60 Hz, 0.5 mA shock was delivered through the grid floor via Shock Scrambler (Bio-Signal Group). Repeated shocks were administered every 1500 ms until the mouse left the shock zone. Each trial lasted 10 min, and was conducted for 5 consecutive days. Track Analysis software (Bio-Signal group, version 2.2) was used to analyse the recorded tracks for each mouse. All mice in our study received tamoxifen to control for effect of the drug on performance [13].

Statistical analyses

Two-way ANOVA was performed, with repeated-measures. Any significant main effect of genotype or day detected by two-way ANOVA was followed by multiple t-tests using a pooled estimate of variance where appropriate with p values reported in the text. Statistical analyses were performed in Prism 7.0 (Graphpad).

Results

In rodents, the generation of adult born hippocampal neurons is required for learning and memory. We previously reported that NfixiNestin mice generate fewer mature dentate granule neurons and consequently display learning deficits in a hippocampal-dependent learning and memory task [7]; (Fig. 1a). We found across the 5 days of the APA task, NfixiNestin mice consistently received the same number of shocks, in contrast to control mice, who received significantly fewer shocks on days 4 and 5. Our initial analysis pooled mice from both sexes. Since many previous studies utilising the APA task typically use only one sex of mouse [2, 6, 8, 9], we therefore first explored if there were any differences in performance in the APA task based on the sex of the mice. When analysing the total number of shocks, we found no sex differences across the 5 days of testing for either control (Fig. 1b) or NfixiNestin mice (Fig. 1c).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Behavioural analyses from the active place avoidance task. a Schematic of the active place avoidance (APA) testing arena. Four black and white visual cues are placed around the platform, which rotated clockwise (blue arrow). The shock zone location is shown in red. b The total number of shocks received by male (dashed line, n = 10) and female (solid line, n = 11) NfixControl mice in the APA task. c The total number of shocks received by male (red dashed line, n = 7) and female (red solid line, n = 14) NfixiNestin mice in the APA task. No significant differences were found in the number of shocks received by male and female mice of either genotype. d Time of first entry to the shock zone was not different in NfixControl mice (black) and NfixiNestin mice (red) during the APA task. e Time to second entrance into the shock zone was significantly delayed in NfixControl mice (black) compared to NfixiNestin mice (red) on day 4 of the APA task. Maximum time shock avoidance (f) and maximum path of shock avoidance (g) during the APA task was not significantly different between NfixControl mice (black) and NfixiNestin mice (red). h The number of entries into the shock zone was significantly higher in NfixiNestin (red) mice on days 4 and 5 of the APA task compared to NfixControl mice (black). i Total time spent in the shock zone was significantly longer for NfixiNestin (red) mice on days 4 and 5 of the APA task compared to NfixControl mice (black). **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05. All graphs depict mean ± s.e.m; n = 21 mice per genotype in (d–i)

Our initial description of the learning and memory phenotype of NfixiNestin mice only reported the total number of shocks received across the 5 days of the APA task. Whilst this analysis provided a measure of the cumulative performance of the mice in the task, it did not specifically assess the long-term encoding and retrieval of spatial memory, which is better addressed by examining latency to first and second shock [8]. We therefore further probed the learning and memory deficit apparent in the NfixiNestin mice by analysing the latency to these shocks. For latency to first shock, we found no difference between the genotypes (Fig. 1d). For latency to second shock, we found improved performance in control mice (F1, 40 = 9.34, p = .003) on day 4 of the APA task compared to NfixiNestin mice (p = .016, Fig. 1e). Interestingly, we found female NfixiNestin mice perform better than their male counterparts for both time to first (p = .008, Additional file 1: Figure S1B) and second shock on day 5 (p < 0.001, Additional file 1: Figure S1D).

To further evaluate the learning and memory capabilities of NfixiNestin mice, we analysed other parameters assessed in the APA task, including the maximum time shock avoidance, the maximum path implemented to avoid a shock, number of entries into the shock zone and time spent in the shock zone. We found no significant differences between control and NfixiNestin mice in the maximum time of shock avoidance (Fig. 1f) and maximum path of shock avoidance (Fig. 1g), suggesting both genotypes form a spatial memory of the location of the shock zone. Why then do NfixiNestin mice consistently receive the same number of shocks across the days of the APA task? We found the total number of entries into the shock zone was significantly higher in NfixiNestin mice (F1, 40 = 9.15, p  = .0043) on days 4 (p  = .031) and 5 (p  = .031) of the task compared to controls (Fig. 1h), and that NfixiNestin mice spend significantly more time in the shock zone (F1, 40 = 10.61, p  = .023) on days 4 (p  = .0033) and 5 (p  = .024) compared to control mice (Fig. 1i). We found no differences between male and female mice of either genotype for these remaining APA parameters (Additional file 1: Figure S1E–L).

Discussion

The APA task is designed to probe spatial learning and memory. Successful avoidance of the shock zone in the APA task relies in part on adult hippocampal neurogenesis [5, 6, 9, 14]. Adult born hippocampal neurons are generated from neural stem cells which reside in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus [15]. We have previously shown that Nfix is expressed by adult neural stem cells, and that in NfixiNestin mice, where Nfix is specifically ablated from adult neural stem cells, fewer adult born neurons are produced and these mice consequently show learning and memory deficits [7]. However, lacking from this study was an appreciation of the extent of the learning and memory deficit, including if there was a memory encoding and retrieval problem. Here, we extended our behavioural analysis of NfixiNestin mice, analysing latency to first and second shock, maximum time shock avoidance, maximum path shock avoidance, number of entries into the shock zone and time spent in the shock zone. Our initial experimental design utilised both male and female mice, whereas many previous APA investigations only use one sex [2, 6, 8, 9]. We could therefore partition the data based on sex, and ask if there were any sex differences apparent in performance in the APA task. We show that there was no difference in the total number of shocks received by male and female mice in either control or NfixiNestin mice. Whilst using only one sex of animal in behavioural studies reduces potential sources of variability, this may not always be feasible when working with transgenic mice. Indeed, a recent commentary highlighted the over-reliance on male mice used for experiments, and how this might potentially bias and affect experimental outcomes and impact the translatability of research to human populations [16]. The results of our study suggest that pooling male and female mice for the APA task could be considered a feasible option, as others have done when working with transgenic mice [17].

We previously reported that NfixiNestin mice move at a similar speed and distance to controls in the APA task [7]. Here we show that the maximum time and path of shock avoidance was similar between the two genotypes, indicating that the reduced performance of NfixiNestin mice in the APA task is not related to movement differences. Furthermore, it is unlikely that altered pain perception can explain our results, as our genetic manipulation of knocking out Nfix from adult neural stem cells is performed in adult mice and is highly specific, and NfixiNestin mice are responsive to tail pinch [7]. Here, we show that the learning and memory deficit in NfixiNestin mice likely relates to their inability to escape the shock zone, with NfixiNestin mice entering the shock zone more frequently and spending more time in the shock zone than control mice. This may be a reflection of the different spatial navigation strategies used by NfixiNestin mice in the APA task. The APA task relies on allocentric spatial navigation, where the rotating arena requires the mouse to continually integrate visual cues to orient itself relative to the room to avoid the fixed shock zone location [2]. In contrast, egocentric navigation relies on cues generated by self-movement, such as proprioceptive cues or cues from optic or auditory flow. Relying on egocentric cues to predict the shock zone could result in poor performance in the APA task, and may explain why NfixiNestin mice spend more time in the shock zone and receive more shocks compared to controls. Whilst previous work suggests male mice perform better in allocentric navigation tasks and females in egocentric navigation tasks [18, 19], we found no differences in maximum time or path of shock avoidance and time spent in the shock zone for control and NfixiNestin mice based on sex. Female NfixiNestin mice did show improved long term memory capabilities compared to their male counterparts, with an increased latency to first and second shock, but this did not translate into fewer shocks received. To provide a more comprehensive understanding of spatial navigation in NfixiNestin mice, future investigations should analyse other forms of spatial learning, including egocentric learning and memory tasks, such as the Cincinnati water maze [20].

The results from our study and previous work [7] affirm the importance of Nfix in broad aspects of adult neural stem cell biology, from their production and survival to its functional importance in learning and memory. Understanding these diverse roles of Nfix will allow insight into human congenital disorders caused by mutations in NFIX, such as Marshall-Smith syndrome and Malan syndrome [21, 22]. These patients have a degree of intellectual disability, including short-term and working memory deficits [23]. Whilst no gross hippocampal abnormalities are reported in these patients [24], subtle changes in hippocampal morphology and altered network dynamics resulting from fewer adult born neurons could contribute to the neurological deficits seen in these patients.

Limitations

Our analysis of learning and memory using the APA task assesses hippocampal-dependent behaviour. Other behavioural assays would provide a more thorough investigation of other forms of spatial learning and memory.

Abbreviations

APA:

active place avoidance

NFIX:

nuclear factor one X

References

  1. Cimadevilla JM, Fenton AA, Bures J. Functional inactivation of dorsal hippocampus impairs active place avoidance in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2000;285:53–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Cimadevilla JM, Fenton AA, Bures J. New spatial cognition tests for mice: passive place avoidance on stable and active place avoidance on rotating arenas. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:559–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Cimadevilla JM, Kaminsky Y, Fenton A, Bures J. Passive and active place avoidance as a tool of spatial memory research in rats. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;102:155–64.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Cimadevilla JM, Wesierska M, Fenton AA, Bures J. Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:3531–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Stuchlik A, Petrasek T, Prokopova I, Holubova K, Hatalova H, Vales K, Kubik S, Dockery C, Wesierska M. Place avoidance tasks as tools in the behavioral neuroscience of learning and memory. Physiol Res. 2013;62(Suppl 1):S1–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Vukovic J, Borlikova GG, Ruitenberg MJ, Robinson GJ, Sullivan RK, Walker TL, Bartlett PF. Immature doublecortin-positive hippocampal neurons are important for learning but not for remembering. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6603–13.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Harris L, Zalucki O, Clement O, Fraser J, Matuzelski E, Oishi S, Harvey TJ, Burne THJ, Heng JI, Gronostajski RM, Piper M. Neurogenic differentiation by hippocampal neural stem and progenitor cells is biased by NFIX expression. Development. 2018;145:dev155689.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Willis EF, Bartlett PF, Vukovic J. Protocol for short- and longer-term spatial learning and memory in mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2017;11:197.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Burghardt NS, Park EH, Hen R, Fenton AA. Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus. 2012;22:1795–808.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Harris L, Zalucki O, Gobius I, McDonald H, Osinki J, Harvey TJ, Essebier A, Vidovic D, Gladwyn-Ng I, Burne TH, et al. Transcriptional regulation of intermediate progenitor cell generation during hippocampal development. Development. 2016;143:4620–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Messina G, Biressi S, Monteverde S, Magli A, Cassano M, Perani L, Roncaglia E, Tagliafico E, Starnes L, Campbell CE, et al. Nfix regulates fetal-specific transcription in developing skeletal muscle. Cell. 2010;140:554–66.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Imayoshi I, Ohtsuka T, Metzger D, Chambon P, Kageyama R. Temporal regulation of Cre recombinase activity in neural stem cells. Genesis. 2006;44:233–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Vogt MA, Chourbaji S, Brandwein C, Dormann C, Sprengel R, Gass P. Suitability of tamoxifen-induced mutagenesis for behavioral phenotyping. Exp Neurol. 2008;211(25–33):17.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Stuchlik A. Dynamic learning and memory, synaptic plasticity and neurogenesis: an update. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:106.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Goncalves JT, Schafer ST, Gage FH. Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus: from stem cells to behavior. Cell. 2016;167:897–914.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Clayton JA, Collins FS. Policy: NIH to balance sex in cell and animal studies. Nature. 2014;509:282–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Leinenga G, Gotz J. Scanning ultrasound removes amyloid-beta and restores memory in an Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. Sci Transl Med. 2015;7:278ra233.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Vorhees CV, Herring NR, Schaefer TL, Grace CE, Skelton MR, Johnson HL, Williams MT. Effects of neonatal (+)-methamphetamine on path integration and spatial learning in rats: effects of dose and rearing conditions. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2008;26:599–610.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Assessing spatial learning and memory in rodents. ILAR J. 2014;55:310–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Cincinnati water maze: a review of the development, methods, and evidence as a test of egocentric learning and memory. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2016;57:1–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Malan V, Rajan D, Thomas S, Shaw AC, Louis Dit Picard H, Layet V, Till M, van Haeringen A, Mortier G, Nampoothiri S, et al. Distinct effects of allelic NFIX mutations on nonsense-mediated mRNA decay engender either a Sotos-like or a Marshall-Smith syndrome. Am J Hum Genet. 2010;87:189–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Sotos JF. Sotos syndrome 1 and 2. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev. 2014;12:2–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Lane C, Milne E, Freeth M. Cognition and behaviour in sotos syndrome: a systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0149189.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. van Balkom ID, Shaw A, Vuijk PJ, Franssens M, Hoek HW, Hennekam RC. Development and behaviour in Marshall-Smith syndrome: an exploratory study of cognition, phenotype and autism. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2011;55:973–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

OZ, LH & MP designed experiments; OZ & LH performed experiments; OZ, DH, LH, TB, RG & MP analysed the data; OZ, DH, MP wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank Daniel Blackmore for advice and guidance on the APA task.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

Data available on request to the corresponding author.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The work performed in this study conformed to The University of Queensland Animal Welfare Unit guidelines for animal use in research (AEC approval numbers QBI/353/13/NHMRC and QBI/383/16). Experiments were performed in alignment with the Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes, and were carried out in accordance with The University of Queensland Institutional Biosafety committee.

Funding

This work was funded by an Australian Research Council (ARC) Discovery Project Grants (DP160100368 and DP180100017 to M.P. and T.H.J.B.) and a New York Stem Cell Foundation Grant (C030133 to R.M.G.). M.P. was supported by an ARC Future Fellowship (FT120100170). L.H. was supported by an Australian Postgraduate Fellowship funded by the Australian Government.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Piper.

Additional file

Additional file 1: Figure S1.

Comparison of male and female mice for additional behavioural analyses from the active place avoidance task. (A) Time of first entry into the shock zone was not different in male (dashed line) and female (solid line) NfixControl mice. (B) Time to first entrance into the shock zone was significantly delayed in female NfixiNestin mice (solid line) compared to male NfixiNestin mice (dashed line) on day 5 of the APA task. (C) Time of second entry into the shock zone was not different in male (dashed line) and female (solid line) NfixControl mice. (D) Time to second entrance into the shock zone was significantly delayed in female NfixiNestin mice (solid line) compared to male NfixiNestin mice (dashed line) on day 5 of the APA task. Maximum time shock avoidance was not significantly different between male (dashed line) and female mice (solid line) for NfixControl mice (black, E) or NfixiNestin mice (red, F). Maximum path shock avoidance was not significantly different between male (dashed line) and female mice (solid line) for NfixControl mice (black, G) or NfixiNestin mice (red, H). The number of entries into the shock zone was not significantly different between male (dashed line) and female mice (solid line) for NfixControl mice (black, I) or NfixiNestin mice (red, J). Time spent in the shock zone was not significantly different between male (dashed line) and female mice (solid line) for NfixControl mice (black, K) or NfixiNestin mice (red, L). ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. All graphs depict mean ± s.e.m; n = 10 male NfixControl mice, n = 11 female NfixControl mice, n = 7 male NfixiNestin mice and n = 14 female NfixiNestin mice.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zalucki, O., Harkins, D., Harris, L. et al. Analysis of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory behaviour in mice lacking Nfix from adult neural stem cells. BMC Res Notes 11, 564 (2018). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13104-018-3652-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13104-018-3652-7

Keywords