Skip to main content

Exploring intervention components in association with changes in preschool children’s vegetable intake: the BRA-study

Abstract

Objective

The present study aimed to explore kindergarten staffs’ perceived usefulness of intervention components in association with changes in children’s vegetable intake and vegetables served in the kindergarten. Assessment of the perceived usefulness of intervention components consisted of a paper-based questionnaire for the kindergarten staff assessing usefulness of posters, supplementary material and 1-day inspirational course. Children’s vegetable intake in the kindergarten was assessed by direct observation, while vegetables served was assessed by a 5-day weighted vegetable diary.

Results

Seventy-three kindergartens in two counties in Norway participated (response rate 15%) and parental consent was obtained for 633 children 3–5 years of age at baseline (response rate 39%). Mixed effect models indicated a tendency that posters were associated with increased child vegetable intake (P = 0.062). Surprisingly, a low degree of perceived usefulness of supplementary material was associated with the largest increase in child vegetable intake (P = 0.020). No significant associations between perceived usefulness of intervention components and vegetables served in the kindergarten were found. This study indicated a tendency that posters were associated with increased child vegetable intake; however, this may also be due to synergies between multiple intervention components.

Trial registration International Standard Randomized Controlled Trials ISRCTN51962956 (http://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN51962956). Registered 21 June 2016 (retrospectively registered).

Introduction

Benefits of sufficient intake of fruits and vegetables are recognized in the prevention of non-communicable diseases and all-cause mortality [1,2,3,4,5]. Despite this, consumption in many countries [5], including Norway [6,7,8,9,10] is low. Consequently, increasing consumption is a public health priority [5].

Childcare settings, called kindergartens in Norway, frequently consist of multiple units, called departments. Each department is staffed with one pedagogical leader in addition to two or more assistants, and includes 18 children, either of the same or of mixed age. Meals are either brought from home (lunch box), provided by the kindergarten or a combination. Kindergarten staff are mainly responsible for the foods served at the lunch meal. This is mostly sandwich with spreads, but a hot meal is served about once a week [11]. There are normative national guidelines for food and meals served in the kindergartens [12] and they specify that vegetables and fruit/berries should be included in all meals.

Several interventions have been conducted to understand how to increase vegetable intake in early childhood [13,14,15]. Results indicate that multicomponent interventions are more successful in increasing vegetable consumption compared to single exposure strategies [13]. Still, knowledge on how to successfully increase vegetable intake is limited. Process evaluations are essential to assist in understanding why or why not an intervention was successful [16,17,18]. Further, it is increasingly recognized that acceptability is an important part of successful implementation [19]. Previous research among schoolchildren has indicated that vegetable intake was higher among those who valued the intervention the most [20, 21]. We hypothesized that perceived usefulness could be considered as a marker of acceptability of the intervention components. The present study therefore aimed to explore the association between kindergarten staffs’ perceived usefulness of the intervention components and changes in children’s vegetable intake and vegetables served in the kindergarten, from baseline to follow-up 1.

Main text

Materials and methods

Study design and subjects have been published elsewhere [22, 23]. Briefly, overall aim of the BRA-study was to improve vegetable intake (primary outcome) among preschool children through changing the food environment and food-related practices in kindergarten and home (secondary outcomes). Child consumption of vegetables and vegetables served in the kindergarten were assessed with regards to frequency, variety and amount of vegetables served and consumed.

Children born in 2010–2011, attending public or private kindergartens in the counties of Vestfold and Buskerud, Norway were included. All regular kindergartens (n = 479) in the counties were invited. Seventy-three kindergartens participated (response rate 15.2%) and parental consent was obtained for 633 children (response rate 38.8%). This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the Norwegian Center for Research Data approved all procedures involving human subjects.

The intervention and its components have been published elsewhere [24, 25]. Briefly, the intervention components to the kindergartens consisted of a 1-day inspirational course (kitchen practice and theory) in addition to resources like aprons, a vegetable memory game, a hand blender, booklets, posters and brochures. The intervention components were related to the four determinants: availability, accessibility, encouragement and role modelling. The intervention focused on changing these determinants, as implemented in the intervention components. The implementation of the intervention was delivered by the kindergarten staff to the children during the daily meals and additional pedagogical activities [24, 25]. The kindergartens were encouraged to change their practices according to their own action plans using the provided training and resources. Posters were for example made to remind staff to serve vegetables (availability, accessibility) and promote (role model, encourage) vegetable intake by the children.

Data collection

There were 313 (49%) children in the intervention group, representing 37 kindergartens and 70 departments. The no-intervention control group continued as normal for the duration of the study and participated only by providing data for the effect analysis.

The research team conducted direct observations to assess children’s vegetable intake at two meals (lunch and an afternoon snack meal) in 1 day in the kindergarten. A 5-day weighted vegetable diary was completed by the kindergarten staff to assess amount of vegetables served in the kindergarten. Data was collected at baseline (spring 2015) and at follow-up 1 (spring 2016) and a description of methods are provided elsewhere [22, 23]. By completing all data collections, kindergartens received a gift card of 2000 NOK (Approx. 223 EUR).

In January 2016, process evaluation was conducted in the intervention group, both in kindergartens and at home. Due to a lack of significant effects upon children’s vegetable consumption in the home setting [25, 26], this paper reports on process evaluation from the kindergarten, including one staff member from each kindergarten/department.

Process evaluation consisted of a paper-based questionnaire including several aspects assessing to what degree the intervention components were perceived useful or not for changing usual vegetable practices in the kindergarten. The present study includes responses to the question “to what degree have the following components been important in getting started with the changes you have been working on?” (i.e., during the intervention period). Perceived usefulness of the components was assessed by four response alternatives: “not at all”, “to a small degree”, “to some degree” and “to a large degree”. Nine intervention components were combined into three groups: posters, supplementary material and 1-day inspirational course (Table 1).

Table 1 Kindergarten staff’s perceived usefulness of the given intervention materials in the BRA-study (n = 48)

Data analysis

Children’s observed vegetable intake (grams/day) and served vegetables in the kindergarten (grams/day) were dependent variables in separate mixed effects models. For children’s observed vegetable intake, kindergarten, department and participant were used as random effects, while for vegetables served in the kindergarten, kindergarten and department were used as random effects. Time (baseline and follow-up 1) and perceived usefulness were used as fixed effects. The reference category for perceived usefulness was “not at all/to a small degree”. To avoid small subgroups, response alternatives “not at all” and “to a small degree” were merged into one category in analysis. We defined the effect of a component, as the difference in change of vegetable intake from baseline to follow-up 1 between the response alternative “to some degree”/“to a large degree” and the reference category. These effects were estimated by the interaction terms between the intervention components and time (Table 2). The perceived usefulness factor thus measured the effect of being in one of the two other categories relative to being in the reference category. A potential difference in effects of the perceived usefulness categories between the two time points may be attributed to the intervention. It was of interest to assess if the intervention provided the same change in effect of “to some degree” and “to a large degree” levels. This was accommodated by an interaction effect between time and perceived usefulness. Test for significance was done by likelihood ratio tests. Corresponding models were considered for each of the two outcomes and each of the three groups of intervention components. Models including children’s vegetable intake were adjusted for covariates: maternal education, child gender and child year of birth. Participants were included in analyses if they had data on baseline and/or follow-up 1 and if they had data on all covariates. All analyses were conducted using Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC. P values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Table 2 Estimated mean vegetable intake (gram) by component and time in the BRA-study

Results

Of the 48 departments (69%) that completed the process evaluation, 34 departments (49%) had complete data on process evaluation and the 5-day weighted vegetable diary at baseline and/or at follow-up 1. Of the 217 children that had been observed at baseline and/or at follow-up 1, 161 children (74%) had data on all covariates and the process evaluation.

Analyses indicated a tendency that in kindergartens where posters were considered useful by the staff, an increase in children’s vegetable intake (P = 0.062) was observed (Table 2). Kindergartens who perceived posters useful “to a large degree” showed an additional increase in children’s vegetable intake of 38 g/day (95% CI 4.6, 70.7) compared to those who perceived posters as “not at all/to a small degree” useful.

Interestingly, in kindergartens where staff reported that supplementary material were perceived as “to some degree” useful, a significantly smaller increase in children’s vegetable intake was observed compared to those who reported perceived usefulness to be “not at all/to a small degree”. A mean difference in children’s vegetable intake of 39 g/day between groups was estimated (95% CI − 65.5, − 12.1) (P = 0.020) (Table 2).

No significant effects were found for the perceived usefulness of the 1-day inspirational course upon children’s vegetable intake (Table 2). Furthermore, no significant effects on amount of vegetables served in the kindergarten setting were found for posters, supplementary material or for the 1-day inspirational course (Table 2).

To assess mean response in the intervention components across time, marginal means were estimated. There seemed to be a trend towards larger increase in children’s vegetable intake with increasing degree of perceived usefulness of posters [“to some degree” (P = 0.03) and “to a large degree” (P < 0.001)] (Fig. 1A).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Estimated marginal means of the BRA-study, showing children’s vegetable intake (n = 161) at baseline and at follow-up 1, adjusted for time, maternal education, child gender and child year of birth in relation to what degree the intervention components (posters, supplementary material and 1-day inspirational course) were perceived useful by the kindergarten staff. (Black box) Baseline data, (Orange dotted box) Follow-up 1 data

For supplementary material, children’s vegetable intake increased by 64 g/day among those who reported perceived usefulness to be “not at all/to a small degree” (P < 0.001) while the increase was 43 g/day for those who reported perceived usefulness to be “to a large degree” (P = 0.002) (Fig. 1B).

For 1-day inspirational course upon children’s vegetable intake, a significant increase of 46–50 g/day were seen in all groups [“not at all/to a small degree” (P = 0.001), “to some degree” (P = 0.018) and “to a large degree” (P < 0.001)] (Fig. 1C).

A significant increase in vegetables served were seen for those who reported perceived usefulness of posters to be “to a large degree” (P = 0.024) (Additional file 1: Figure S1A). No significant increases were observed for supplementary material and 1-day inspirational course (Additional file 1: Figure S1B, C).

Discussion

Findings suggest that there was a trend towards larger increase in children’s vegetable intake with increasing degree of perceived usefulness of posters. However, this may also be due to synergies between intervention components. This is in line with previously reported associations for child vegetable intake and appreciation for an intervention [20, 21].

When kindergartens were visited at follow-up 1, the impression was that many of the intervention kindergartens had displayed the posters. Hence, posters might have had a prompting effect [27] and been a reinforcement of the key messages on amount to be served and how to encourage children to taste and eat vegetables. As an illustration, the two posters on amount of vegetables might have been especially important in creating shifts in children’s vegetable intake as they displayed suggestions for children’s vegetable portion sizes. Studies among adults have reported confusion over portion sizes for fruits and vegetables [28, 29]. This might also be the case here, as the adult recommended intake of fruits and vegetables is 500 g/day [30] in Norway, while there is no precise recommendations for amount for young children.

It is difficult to explain why those who reported the lowest degree of perceived usefulness of supplementary material showed a significantly larger increase in children’s vegetable intake compared to those who perceived such components useful “to some degree”. The hand blender and the aprons should encourage staff to involve children in mealtime tasks. However, 55% of Norwegian kindergartens report to include children in those tasks [11] and thus they might not be perceived the most useful of the components included.

Limitations

Strengths and limitations

As evidence of how to increase vegetable intake in early childhood is scare, this study sought to increase knowledge within this field. One limitation was that process evaluations were completed in January 2016, while the follow-up assessments of vegetables were assessed between April–June 2016. Hence, there was a variation in length of implementation period. For vegetables served in the kindergarten, only half of the eligible sample had complete data and it is not known to what extent this sample was representative of the participating kindergartens. Moreover, different persons may have filled in the 5-day weighted vegetable diary at baseline and at follow-up 1, leading to impaired consistency between measures of vegetables served in the kindergarten [26].

Availability of data and materials

Data used and analyzed during the current study will be available from the corresponding author upon request, provided compliance with current legislation for application for data access in Norway.

Abbreviations

The BRA-study:

An acronym for the Norwegian words “Barnehage” (kindergarten), “gRønnsaker” (vegetables) and “fAmilie” (family). The Norwegian word “BRA” means “good”

References

  1. Boeing H, Bechthold A, Bub A, et al. Critical review: vegetables and fruit in the prevention of chronic diseases. Eur J Nutr. 2012;51(6):637–63.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Ness AR, Maynard M, Frankel S, et al. Diet in childhood and adult cardiovascular and all cause mortality: the Boyd Orr cohort. Heart. 2005;91(7):894–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Wang X, Ouyang Y, Liu J, et al. Fruit and vegetable consumption and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer: systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 2014;349:g4490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. World Cancer Research Fund, American Institute for Cancer Research. Continous update project expert report 2018—recommendations and public health and policy implications. 2018.

  5. Global strategy on diet, physical activity and health—promoting fruit and vegetable consumption around the world. https://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/fruit/en/. Accessed 13 Feb 2019.

  6. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Spedkost 2 (12-months-olds). IS-1635; 2009.

  7. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Småbarnskost 2 (2-year-olds). IS-1731; 2009.

  8. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Norkost 3 (adults). IS-2000; 2012.

  9. Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Ungkost 3—dietary intake among 4-year-olds; 2016.

  10. Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Ungkost 3—dietary intake among 9- and 13-year-olds; 2016.

  11. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Meals, physical activity and environmental health care in kindergartens. IS-0345; 2012.

  12. National guidelines for food and meals in the kindergarten (in Norwegian). https://www.helsedirektoratet.no/retningslinjer/mat-og-maltider-i-barnehagen. Accessed 6 April 2019.

  13. Hodder RK, O’Brien KM, Tzelepis F, et al. Interventions for increasing fruit and vegetable consumption in children aged five years and under. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;5(5):Cd008552.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Holley CE, Farrow C, Haycraft E. A systematic review of methods for increasing vegetable consumption in early childhood. Curr Nutr Rep. 2017;6(2):157–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Nekitsing C, Blundell-Birtill P, Cockroft JE, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis of strategies to increase vegetable consumption in preschool children aged 2–5 years. Appetite. 2018;127:138–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Baranowski T, Anderson C, Carmack C. Mediating variable framework in physical activity interventions. How are we doing? How might we do better? Am J Prev Med. 1998;15(4):266–97.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Moore GF, Audrey S, Barker M, et al. Process evaluation of complex interventions: medical research council guidance. BMJ. 2015;350:h1258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Steckler A, Linnan L. Process evaluation for public health interventions and research. 1st ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Sekhon M, Cartwright M, Francis JJ. Acceptability of healthcare interventions: an overview of reviews and development of a theoretical framework. BMC Health Serv Res. 2017;17(1):88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Bere E, Veierød MB, Bjelland M, et al. Outcome and process evaluation of a Norwegian school-randomized fruit and vegetable intervention: fruits and vegetables make the marks (FVMM). Health Educ Res. 2006;21(2):258–67.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Wind M, Bjelland M, Pérez-Rodrigo C, et al. Appreciation and implementation of a school-based intervention are associated with changes in fruit and vegetable intake in 10- to 13-year old schoolchildren–the pro children study. Health Educ Res. 2008;23(6):997–1007.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Himberg-Sundet A, Kristiansen AL, Bjelland M, et al. Is the environment in kindergarten associated with the vegetables served and eaten? The BRA study. Scand J Public Health. 2018;47(5):538–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kristiansen AL, Bjelland M, Himberg-Sundet A, et al. Associations between physical home environmental factors and vegetable consumption among Norwegian 3–5-year-olds: the BRA-study. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(7):1173–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Himberg-Sundet A, Kristiansen AL, Frost Andersen L, et al. Effects of a kindergarten intervention on vegetables served and staff’s food-related practices: results of a cluster randomised controlled trial—the BRA study. Public Health Nutr. 2020;23(6):1117–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Kristiansen AL, Bjelland M, Himberg-Sundet A, et al. Effects of a cluster randomized controlled kindergarten-based intervention trial on vegetable consumption among Norwegian 3-5-year-olds: the BRA-study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):1098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Kristiansen AL, Medin AC, Bjelland M, et al. Long-term effects of a cluster randomized controlled kindergarten-based intervention trial on vegetable intake among Norwegian 3–5-year-olds: the BRA-study. BMC Res Notes. 2020;13(1):30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Broers VJV, De Breucker C, Van den Broucke S, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of nudging to increase fruit and vegetable choice. Eur J Public Health. 2017;27(5):912–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Pollard CM, Daly AM, Binns CW. Consumer perceptions of fruit and vegetables serving sizes. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(5):637–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Rooney C, McKinley MC, Appleton KM, et al. How much is ‘5-a-day’? A qualitative investigation into consumer understanding of fruit and vegetable intake guidelines. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2017;30(1):105–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Norwegian guidelines on diet, nutrition and physical activity. IS-2170; 2014.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all children, their parents, and the kindergarten staff who participated in the BRA-study.

Funding

The Research Council of Norway (Project Number 228452) with supplementary funds from the Throne Holst Nutrition Research Foundation, University of Oslo, supported this work. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ALK wrote the draft of the manuscript and AHS conducted the statistical analyses. ALK, MB, NL, and LFA participated in designing the BRA-study. ALK, MB, AHS, NL, and LFA participated in project planning and ALK and AHS participated in data collections. RH assisted and provided advice of statistical analysis. All authors have critically revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne Lene Kristiansen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the Norwegian Center for Research Data approved all procedures involving human subjects. Parental written consent was obtained for 633 children.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Figure S1.

Estimated marginal means of the BRA-study, showing vegetables served in the kindergarten departments (n = 34) at baseline and follow-up 1, in relation to what degree the intervention components (posters, supplementary material and 1-day inspirational course) were perceived useful by the kindergarten staff. (Black box) Baseline data, (Orange dotted box) Follow-up 1 data.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kristiansen, A.L., Himberg-Sundet, A., Bjelland, M. et al. Exploring intervention components in association with changes in preschool children’s vegetable intake: the BRA-study. BMC Res Notes 14, 214 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13104-021-05629-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13104-021-05629-1

Keywords